Elsevier

Neuroscience

Volume 226, 13 December 2012, Pages 227-235
Neuroscience

Binaural speech processing in individuals with auditory neuropathy

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.08.054Get rights and content

Abstract

Auditory neuropathy disrupts the neural representation of sound and may therefore impair processes contingent upon inter-aural integration. The aims of this study were to investigate binaural auditory processing in individuals with axonal (Friedreich ataxia) and demyelinating (Charcot–Marie–Tooth disease type 1A) auditory neuropathy and to evaluate the relationship between the degree of auditory deficit and overall clinical severity in patients with neuropathic disorders.

Twenty-three subjects with genetically confirmed Friedreich ataxia and 12 subjects with Charcot–Marie–Tooth disease type 1A underwent psychophysical evaluation of basic auditory processing (intensity discrimination/temporal resolution) and binaural speech perception assessment using the Listening in Spatialized Noise test. Age, gender and hearing-level-matched controls were also tested.

Speech perception in noise for individuals with auditory neuropathy was abnormal for each listening condition, but was particularly affected in circumstances where binaural processing might have improved perception through spatial segregation. Ability to use spatial cues was correlated with temporal resolution suggesting that the binaural-processing deficit was the result of disordered representation of timing cues in the left and right auditory nerves. Spatial processing was also related to overall disease severity (as measured by the Friedreich Ataxia Rating Scale and Charcot–Marie–Tooth Neuropathy Score) suggesting that the degree of neural dysfunction in the auditory system accurately reflects generalized neuropathic changes.

Measures of binaural speech processing show promise for application in the neurology clinic. In individuals with auditory neuropathy due to both axonal and demyelinating mechanisms the assessment provides a measure of functional hearing ability, a biomarker capable of tracking the natural history of progressive disease and a potential means of evaluating the effectiveness of interventions.

Highlights

► We examine binaural processing in individuals with auditory neuropathy. ► Spatial listening was impaired in subjects with Friedreich ataxia. ► Spatial listening was impaired in subjects with Charcot–Marie–Tooth disease. ► Auditory pathway dysfunction reflected generalized neuropathic changes.

Introduction

The auditory system is exquisitely sensitive. Accurate representation of complex and continually varying acoustic signals (such as speech), requires a high degree of precision. Subtle changes affecting any point in the peripheral or central auditory mechanisms can have a significant impact on perception. The consequences of cochlear damage on sound detection and the processing of (particularly) frequency information have been explored in detail over the past 40 years (Moore, 1995). More recently, the effects of auditory neuropathies on the transmission of electrical signals through the auditory neural pathways (and the consequent effects on functional hearing) have also been considered (Starr et al., 1991, Starr et al., 1996, Rance et al., 2004, Zeng et al., 2005).

The term “auditory neuropathy” (AN) describes a group of hearing disorders in which cochlear (outer hair cell) function is normal, but afferent conduction through the auditory nerve and central pathways is disrupted (Starr et al., 1996). This disruption may be the result of reduced neuronal population (axonopathy) or impaired neural function due to demyelination or abnormal synaptic transmission (Starr et al., 1996). Auditory neuropathy may present in infancy, when it is typically the result of perinatal insult such as oxygen deprivation or hyperbilirubinaemia (Rance et al., 1999), or at a later stage when associated with generalized neurodegenerative processes (Starr et al., 1996, Starr et al., 2000, Rapin and Gravel, 2003).

Friedreich ataxia (FRDA) is one such neurodegenerative disease which affects both motor and sensory systems through mutations in the FXN gene (Campuzano et al., 1996). Physical manifestations of the disease include limb and trunk ataxia, scoliosis, hypertrophic cardiomyopathy and an increased rate of diabetes mellitus. Sensory deficits have been observed across multiple modalities including the visual, vestibular systems and auditory systems (Fahey et al., 2008, Rance et al., 2008, Fielding et al., 2010). Auditory neuropathy occurs in a high proportion of cases (>90% in the latter disease stages [Rance et al., 2008, Rance et al., 2010a]) as a result of axonopathy in the cochlear nerve (Spoendlin, 1974).

Charcot–Marie–Tooth (CMT) disease is another neurodegenerative syndrome with auditory neuropathy as a phenotypic variant (Satya-Murti et al., 1979, Starr et al., 1996, Kovach et al., 2002). The disease presents with a range of symptoms reflecting peripheral nerve dysfunction including weakness, muscle atrophy, orthopaedic abnormality and sensory deficits. CMT is subdivided into demyelinating forms (dominantly inherited [CMT type 1] and recessively inherited [CMT type 4], axonal forms (generally dominantly inherited [CMT type 2]) and intermediate forms including X-linked varieties. Auditory neuropathy is relatively common in both axonal and demyelinating forms. Prevalence in affected adults is yet to be determined, but recent evidence in school-aged children has suggested that >85% of individuals with CMT2 and >60% with CMT disease type 1A (CMT1A) show abnormal auditory-evoked potentials and impaired perception (Rance et al., 2012b).

A cardinal feature of auditory neuropathy is impaired temporal resolution – the ability to perceive rapid signal changes over time (Starr et al., 1991, Starr et al., 1996, Rance et al., 2004, Zeng et al., 2005). In both demyelinating and axonal forms, this is thought due to neural desynchrony producing a time-smeared representation of the acoustic signal. In the case of demyelinating neuropathy, desynchrony occurs when damage to the neural insulator produces slow/inconsistent conduction and an impaired ability to transmit trains of pulses (Brown and Watson, 2002). In axonal disorders desynchrony may be the result of irregular firing patterns in partially damaged fibres, secondary demyelination or conduction block (Brown and Watson, 2002). Degree of temporal-processing deficit may be equally severe in both AN-forms (Rance et al., 2012b).

The functional consequence of temporal processing disorder is impaired speech perception (particularly in background noise). Monaural (individual ear) tests of speech understanding have shown that most listeners with auditory neuropathy struggle to understand speech even in low levels of noise. This “figure-ground” deficit is thought to be due to a combination of increased masking effects and an inability to use brief quiet periods (in a fluctuating noise) to access the speech signal (Zeng et al., 2005, Rance et al., 2007, Rance et al., 2010b).

Temporal-processing deficit may also affect a listener’s ability to combine signals from the two ears to maximize perception. When sound emanates from any point other than directly in front of the listener (0° azimuth), the signal travels an unequal distance to each ear. This gives rise to subtle (<1 ms) inter-aural time differences (ITDs). Furthermore, the head acts as a barrier when the sound source is not directly in front which results in inter-aural level differences (ILDs) of approximately 5–15 dB. These timing and intensity differences are initially processed in the lower brainstem where neural impulses originating in the left and right auditory nerves meet at the superior olivary complex (SOC) and are converted to location (and movement) information (Reidel and Kollmeier, 2002) before being relayed to higher centres in the auditory pathway (Spitzer and Semple, 1993, Riedel and Kollmeier, 2002, Warren et al., 2002, Kuwada et al., 2006). As a result, listeners with normal hearing may perceive a three-dimensional listening environment and be able to accurately judge the direction of sound sources. This capacity has obvious benefits alerting the listener to possible information sources and environmental dangers. In addition, sound localization cues may also be used to improve perception in background noise when a target signal (such as speech) and competing noise arise from different directions. That is, acoustic sequences from different locations may be parsed into separate “streams” and a target stream may be selectively attended as a separate entity within a complex signal (Micheyl et al., 2007, Cameron and Dillon, 2008).

Auditory neuropathy disrupts the neural representation of sound and may therefore impair processes contingent upon inter-aural comparison. Binaural auditory processing has not been explored in detail for listeners with AN, but preliminary evidence suggests that spatial perception is disrupted. Zeng et al. (2005) measured sound localization ability in three affected subjects using a range of binaural stimuli and found that while localization based upon inter-aural level differences was unimpaired, the AN listeners showed little or no ability to use inter-aural timing cues to judge sound direction.

This study had three main aims: (1) to investigate binaural auditory-processing deficits in individuals with axonal and demyelinating auditory neuropathy; (2) to evaluate the relationship between degree of auditory-processing deficit and overall clinical severity in patients with neurodegenerative disease; (3) to track binaural processing changes over time in affected individuals.

Section snippets

Experimental procedures

This study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Royal Victorian Eye and Ear Hospital, Melbourne, Australia. Written consent was obtained from all participants following explanation of the nature, purpose and expected outcomes of the research.

Friedreich ataxia

Detection of intensity differences was unimpaired in subjects with Friedreich ataxia. Mean intensity-level difference for the FRDA group was 5.0 ± 1.0 dB and for the controls was 5.1 ± 0.9 dB (P > 0.05).

Similarly, detection of slow (10 Hz) AM changes (which at this rate is essentially an intensity discrimination task) was also normal in FRDA subjects (FRDA: −20.9 ± 7.1 dB [9.0% of maximum amplitude]; Control: −22.1 ± 5.1 dB [7.8% of maximum], P > 0.05). In contrast, identification of rapid (150 Hz) amplitude

Discussion

The findings of this study suggest that temporal distortion in the left and right auditory nerves of individuals with AN disrupts integration of binaural difference cues. This, in turn affects the listener’s ability to use spatial information to improve speech perception in background noise. Furthermore, the data suggest that changes occurring in the auditory pathway reflect overall disease progression in individuals with neurodegenerative disorder.

The electrophysiologic findings clearly

Competing interests statement

The authors declare that they have no competing financial interests.

Funding

This work was supported by the Friedreich Ataxia Research Alliance (FARA USA) and the Friedreich Ataxia Research Association (FARA Australasia). G.R. holds the Wagstaff Research Fellowship in Otolaryngology. M.B.D. is a NHMRC Practitioner Fellow.

Acknowledgements

We would like to express our gratitude to the participants who gave so freely of their time. Thanks also to Dr. Sharon Cameron and Dr. Harvey Dillon from the National Acoustics Laboratories for their technical advice.

References (41)

  • W.F. Brown et al.

    Pathophysiology of conduction in peripheral neuropathies

  • J. Burns et al.

    Validation of the Charcot–Marie–Tooth disease pediatric scale as an outcome measure of disability

    Ann Neurol

    (2012)
  • S. Cameron et al.

    Development of the listening in spatialized noise-sentences test (LiSN-S)

    Ear Hear

    (2007)
  • S. Cameron et al.

    The Listening in Spatialized Noise-Sentences Test (LISN-S): comparison to the prototype LISN and results from children with either a suspected (central) auditory processing disorder or a confirmed language disorder

    J Am Acad Audiol

    (2008)
  • V. Campuzano et al.

    Friedreich’s ataxia: autosomal recessive disease caused by an intronic GAA triplet repeat expansion

    Science

    (1996)
  • H.E. Cullington et al.

    Speech recognition with varying numbers and types of competing talkers by normal-hearing, cochlear-implant, and implant simulation subjects

    J Acoust Soc Am

    (2008)
  • M.B. Delatycki

    Evaluating the progression of Friedreich ataxia and its treatment

    J Neurol

    (2009)
  • M.F. Fahey et al.

    Vestibular, saccadic and fixation abnormalities in genetically confirmed Friedreich ataxia

    Brain

    (2008)
  • M.J. Kovach et al.

    Anticipation in a unique family with Charcot–Marie–Tooth syndrome and deafness: delineation of the clinical features and review of the literature

    Am J Med Gen

    (2002)
  • N. Kraus et al.

    Consequences of neural asynchrony: a case of auditory neuropathy

    J Assoc Res Otolaryngol

    (2000)
  • Cited by (32)

    • Task force Guideline of Brazilian Society of Otology ‒ hearing loss in children – Part I ‒ Evaluation

      2023, Brazilian Journal of Otorhinolaryngology
      Citation Excerpt :

      There are other hereditary progressive motor and sensory neuropathies with auditory neuropathy, such as Friedreich ataxia, with possible spiral ganglion degeneration. Cochlear implantation is likely to have suboptimal postoperative results in these individuals, but it is still a treatment option for deafness.101 Mohr-Tranebjaerg syndrome (deafness-dystonia syndrome) with optic neuropathy is a progressive genetic neurodegenerative disease characterized by childhood-onset auditory neuropathy, dystonia, and ataxia beginning in the second decade of life, with decreased hearing acuity beginning in the third decade and dementia in the fifth decade of life.

    • Friedreich ataxia

      2020, Rosenberg’s Molecular and Genetic Basis of Neurological and Psychiatric Disease: Volume 2
    • Rapid exhaustion of auditory neural conduction in a prototypical mitochondrial disease, Friedreich ataxia

      2018, Clinical Neurophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Besides the observation of the peculiar ABR pattern in 38% patients, the features of the present sample of FRDA patients, in terms of speech performance and ABRs, are largely comparable to previously reported samples of FRDA patients. For example, absent or severely distorted ABRs were described in 3 of 10 cases (Rance et al., 2008), 9 of 14 cases (Rance et al., 2010), and 11 of 19 cases (Rance et al., 2012a), and when present with normal waveforms, ABRs sometimes had a wave V/I peak-to-peak amplitude ratio below the normal range (Rance et al., 2012a). The detection of speech in noise was widely variable among subjects, and rather weakly correlated with the overall severity of the disease and with ABR category.

    • Auditory neuropathy in a patient with hemochromatosis

      2016, Journal of Otology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Auditory findings for Patient DM were compared with data from 80 healthy control subjects with sound detection thresholds in the mild hearing loss range (4 frequency average: 24.7 ± 5.2 dBHL). Selected results for these individuals (39 female) have been published previously (Rance et al., 2012a, 2012b; 2014). Age at assessment for the group ranged from 10 years to 76 years (52.2 ± 14.9 years).

    • Auditory deficits of Kcna1 deletion are similar to those of a monaural hearing impairment

      2015, Hearing Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Given the known importance for Kv1.1 for coincidence detection in the auditory system, it is conceivable that Kv1.1 channels are necessary for encoding the temporal fine structure of acoustic signals which is especially important for binaural hearing. Although Kv1.1 containing channels are strongly expressed in fast firing neurons of the auditory periphery and lower brainstem, temporal processing deficits caused by malfunctioning of these channels are likely to decrease a listener's ability to combine information from the two: for example, inaccuracies in perceptual grouping based on incorrect temporal integration of auditory signals have been described for other forms of ataxia (Rance et al., 2008, 2012) and are likely to apply to all forms of neural integration that rely on the synchrony or temporal precision of the incoming signals. In the present study we test whether the lack of Kv1.1 channel subunits can account for perceptual deficits similar to those acquired by the loss of binaural hearing.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text